12 April 2012

Still a mystery how pigeons find home

Agence France Presse
Scientists have sent robot scouts into deep space and unravelled the genome, yet they are still baffled by how homing pigeons navigate.
Homing pigeons

PARIS: Scientists have sent robot scouts into deep space and unravelled the genome, yet they are still baffled by how homing pigeons navigate.

Experts at Vienna’s Institute of Molecular Pathology in Austria said they had overturned claims that the birds’ feat is due to iron-rich nerve cells in the beak that are sensitive to Earth’s magnetic field.

After years of endeavour, “it was really disappointing,” said molecular biologist and lead author David Keays. “The mystery of how animals detect magnetic fields has just got more mysterious.”

Navigation not in pigeon’s beak

Keays’ team used 3-D scanners to search for the cells and sliced pigeon beaks into 250,000 wafer-thin slivers for analysis.

They found that the particles credited with the pigeon’s homing skills were actually macrophages, a type of white blood cell which protects the birds from infection and had no connection to the brain.

“They are not excitable cells and cannot produce electric signals which could be registered by neurons (brain cells) and therefore influence the pigeon’s behaviour,” the researchers said.

Nor are these cells exclusive to the beak.

Pigeon’s detect magnetic field…but how?

Keays described the process as “extremely frustrating” but insisted the findings should not be seen as a setback.

“It puts us on the right path to finding magnetic cells,” said Keays.

“It is very clear that birds and a large number of other species detect the Earth’s magnetic field, so they must have a population of cells somewhere that allow them to do this… Hopefully now we can find the real ones.”
Other theories suggest the birds also get a navigational fix from sunlight or from landmarks.

Magnetoreceptors hard to find

Keays said magnetoreceptors were so hard to find because they were so small, probably about 20 to 40 nanometres (20 to 40 billionths of a metre), and “could be anywhere in the pigeon”.

“Trying to find a magnetoreceptor is not like trying to find a needle in a haystack, it’s like trying to find a needle in a haystack of needles.”

Finding it would not only solve a stubborn puzzle but may also have a medical use, he added.

“If we can learn how nature detects magnetic fields we can use that information to create artificial magnetoreceptors that might have some applications in the treatment of disease”, particularly of the brain.

Claim still controversial

As for the other scientists: “I don’t think they are going to like me very much,” Keays admitted.

One of the authors of the original study published in 2000, Günther Fleissner from the University of Frankfurt, fired an angry rebuttal.

He accused Keays’s team of lacking thoroughness in the search for cells in the upper beak, called sensory dendrites, that – if behavioural research is right – can detect a magnetic field.

“It is in fact a hard job to find the iron-containing dendrites, and therefore it is not astonishing at all that these authors obviously have overlooked them,” he said.

“The paper seems to be biased by the authors’ preferred view that these dendrites in reality are macrophages only.”

The new paper appears in the British science journal Nature this week.

NEWSLETTER

Sign up to our free newsletter and have "This Week in Cosmos" delivered to your inbox every Monday.

>> More information
Latest
issue
CONNECT
Like us on Facebook
Follow @CosmosMagazine
Add Cosmos to your Google+ circles

Get a weekly dose of Cosmos delivered straight to your inbox!

  • The latest in science each week
  • All the updates on our new website launch
  • Exclusive offers and competitions

Enter your name and email address below: